In general, drivers drive their vehicles safely, carrying out the procedures of perception, judgement and operation.
  Perception consists of drivers identifying and becoming aware of irregularity and danger in surrounding traffic conditions, and judgement of taking decisions on how to react to the situation perceived. Execution of driving operations in accordance with that judgement is generally termed operation. However, we elected to include within "judgement" in the above system of categorization the predictive behavior of defensive driving consisting of the judgement that, while no danger may presently be apparent at an intersection, someone might appear from around a corner, and to treat this category as one of judgement and prediction.

We refer to the principal party in an accident as Party A and to the other party in a collision as Party B. In other words, all accidents that are not single-vehicle accidents involve another party in the collision (including pedestrians and other vehicles). Of the 300 accidents in 1997, for example, 75 were single-vehicle accidents and so there were 300 Parties A and 225 Parties B.

Figure 1 -- Frequency of Errors, by Party to Accident

  Figure 1 shows, of the 300 Parties A and the 225 Parties B, what proportions committed some sort of error. 298 Parties A, or almost the total number, and 196 Parties B, or 87% of the total, committed an error. Errors of perception (oversight) were most common among the Parties A,indicating that the most effective way to prevent accidents would be to improve their ability to perceive (identify) signs of danger. Unlike the Parties A, the Parties B exhibited a greater proportion of errors of judgement and prediction than of errors of perception. That is, it would seem to be of greater importance among Parties B to judge and predict situations correctly or, in other words, to make judgements and predictions that lead to safer driving, to take care to drive defensively.

Nor do these errors occur singly. As shown in Figures 2 and 3, in the majority of accidents errors of judgement or prediction and errors of operation occur together with errors of perception. For reference, Figure 4 shows the numbers of errors committed per party involved in an accident. Parties A committed around three errors each, and Parties B around two errors each. In other words, the Parties A had around three opportunities to avoid the accidents, and the Parties B around two such opportunities.

Figure 2 -- Dominant Circumstance of Party A Errors Figure 3 -- Dominant Circumstance of Party B Errors


Figure 4 -- Errors per Party to Accident N.B. Definitions of straight-line intersection and oncoming approaches
 1.1 Errors of Perception: Carelessness and mistaken assumptions
 1.2 Errors of Decision-making and Prediction: Mistaken assumptions
 1.3 Errors of Operation: Flurry, panic and mistaken assumptions
UP

1.1 Errors of Perception: Carelessness and mistaken assumptions
  Figure 5 summarizes the subjects of errors of perception by Parties A and the factors involved in their errors of perception. The most common of their errors of perception is failure to notice a traveller on a perpendicular path1, i.e. a vehicle or person coming along a transverse road at an intersection, an error that leads to sideswipe collisions. The second most common error involves a party on a head-on path1 and leads to right-turn collisions (those between a vehicle turning right and another coming head-on). Among factors contributing to errors committed, carelessness and mistaken assumptions were common across all accident types: "I would have seen it if I looked, but I didn't". Errors of perception involving mistaken assumptions are failures to confirm safety, as when a driver knows that there is a stop sign at an intersection and so the other cars will always stop or that "traffic is light on this road and cars hardly ever travel this way". The next most common factors were obstructions of view, an inability to see other traffic due to the shadows of buildings or trees or due to rain or fog: "I wouldn't have seen it even on the lookout". Such idiosyncratic factors contributing to errors committed as falling asleep at the wheel and drunk driving were contributors primarily to lane wandering and deserve particular attention because they lead to such serious accidents as single-vehicle accidents and head-on collisions.
 Figure 6 gives the circumstances of errors of perception by Parties B. Travellers on perpendicular paths represent an overwhelmingly large number,substantiating the large number of sideswipe collisions. The majority of contributing factors to errors committed by Parties B were also mistaken assumptions and obstructions of view.

Figure 5 -- Errors of Perception and Contributing Factors (Parties A)

Figure 6 -- Errors of Perception and Contributing Factors (Parties B)
UP

1.2 Errors of Decision-making and Prediction: Mistaken assumptions
Next, we consider errors of decision-making and prediction and the factors contributing to those errors.
  Figure 7 summarizes the main results obtained for Parties A. The most common error of decision-making and prediction is that despite being aware of an intersection, they saw no other vehicle and so assumed "no one would be on the intersecting road", another mistaken assumption that leads to sideswipe collisions. It serves to look back at Figure 5 at this point. Although travellers on a perpendicular path are often overlooked, intersections per se are rarely overlooked. This means that there is not a little possibility that errors of perception with respect to travellers on a perpendicular path could be resolved by making the safety-oriented decision-making and prediction that "there could be someone on the intersecting road", i.e. taking care to drive defensively, at the point of perceiving an intersection, rather than assuming "no one would be on the intersecting road". What is important is the need, whatever contributing factors may obtain and even when an error of perception obtains, of looking at another object and predicting the presence of objects that were not perceived. The attitude that "my own car's speed is not a problem", which leads to single-vehicle accidents, is present is about the same number, and both suggest decision-makings and predictions that are subjectively convenient. Mistaken assumptions are everywhere in factors contributing to errors, accounting for 60% of the total: "I've got the right of way so no one's going to intrude", for example, or "I've got the green light so no one's going to intrude".

Figure 7 -- Errors of Decision-making and Prediction and Contributing Factors(Parties A)

  On the other hand, as we see in Figure 8, the most common error of decision-making and prediction by Parties B was the assumption, on seeing an intersection, that "no one's going to obstruct my path", followed by the assumption, on seeing a traffic signal, that "no one's going to ignore the signals". The contributing factor leading to almost all such errors is mistaken assumptions. The point is that there is a need to take care to drive defensively in light of a recognition of the fact that not everybody obeys signals and other traffic regulations. Even so, 8% of all contributing factors to errors by Parties B consist of irresistance, in which the Party B could pursue no other course. In short, the significant contributing factor in errors of decision-making and prediction is, as in errors of perception, mistaken assumptions on the part of the driver and need not be remarked on particularly here.
Figure 8 -- Errors of Decision-making and Prediction and Contributing Factors(Parties B)
UP

1.3 Errors of Operation: Flurry, panic and mistaken assumptions
  There are not many errors of operation, but for reference purposes Figure 9 gives errors of operation by Parties A and the factors contributing to those errors. Almost all of the errors involve the steering wheel; the factors contributing to them were, in order of frequency, insufficient driving ability, flurry and panic, drunk driving, overconfidence in control of vehicle, and falling asleep at the wheel. The total number of errors of operation by Parties B is a very low nine and so no figure is presented. The most common such error was three instances of headlights off, followed by two each of unsuitable operation of steering wheel and brake.The factors contributing to these errors were, in order of frequency, flurry and panic, and mistaken assumptions. In short, while there are few errors of operation per se, the fact cannot be overlooked that most of these involve a contributing factor attributable to the driver in which a Party A commits an error using the steering wheel.
Figure 9 -- Errors of Operation and Contributing Factors (Parties A)
UP

Institute for Traffic Accident Research and Data Analysis (ITARDA)